The Effect Of HIIT On Children’s Motor Skills

Kids running under a parachute
November 15th, 2018 0 Comments

Catherine O’Brien

In my last article, I introduced a study that utilized interval training (FUNtervals) in the classroom (Ma, Mare, & Gurd, 2015). The research showed that participating in FUNtervals had the potential to improve selective memory in children. Understanding that attention is the basis for deeper cognitive function (learning, memory etc.), I was interested in looking into research examining how intervals may affect other cognitive processes.

The research was done by Lundbye-Jensen, Skriver, Nielsen, and Roig (2017). They investigated the effects of acute exercise on motor memory in 3rd and 4th-grade students. The aim of the study was to identify whether an acute bout of exercise following a motor skill task in the classroom could improve the retention of motor memory. Previous research has demonstrated a positive relationship between motor skill function and academic achievement and between daily physical education and improved motor skill performance.

As previously discussed, interval training has been shown to produce many of the same beneficial cognitive effects as extended aerobic exercise. Given the previous research in the space, the present researchers designed a study to measure the effects of two types of high-intensity training on motor memory in children.

Participants were 78 diverse children in 3rd or 4th grade. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions- running (RUN), floorball (FLB) or control (CON). In order to help control for differences between the groups, each of the three groups were matched on gender, age, BMI, and fitness level. Throughout the experiment, the subjects participated in 4 separate sessions.

4 Sessions:

  • First Session: pre-examination session including aerobic fitness testing (measuring VO2max)
  • Second Session: Subjects practiced the motor memory task
  • Third Session: Participants engaged in running (RUN), floorball (FLB) or nothing (CON) based on his or her assigned condition for 20 minutes. This session took place immediately after the practice motor task session. *
  • Fourth Session: Retention tests were run 1-hour, 24-hours and 7-days post initial practice of motor task.

*All subjects rested for 40 minutes upon completion of the intervention (RUN, FLB, CON).

The motor memory task was designed using customized software. Participants were seated in front of a computer screen and asked to control the mouse with his or her dominant hand. The mouse moved vertically and horizontally. After one minute of familiarization with the feel of the cursor and the associated movement of the arrow on the screen, the task initiated. In this task, subjects were told to match the cursor as accurately as possible to the target. All targets required that the child move the cursor both vertically and horizontally.

Related Article: Children Weight Lifting: What You Need To Know!

Motor performance was measured based on the root mean square error (RMSE) between the preset target and the subjects’ trace. Between each of the 8 targets presented, the cursor was positioned at “target mean” in the middle of the screen. In this way, subjects always started and ended at the same central location. “Their score was defined as the mean of absolute vertical errors between the cursor and target in relation to target mean. If the error exceeded two times the distance from target to target mean, the score for this data point was set to zero.” (p.183).

Three Rounds:

Each subject completed blocks of 24 trials including three rounds of the eight different targets. Each trial was followed by a pause in which the subject received feedback regarding:

  1. Knowledge of result – i.e. their score from 0-100
  2. Knowledge of performance- which included a picture that showed the subjects’ trace and the target and
  3. Average scores so children understood how his or her scores compared to the average performance.

In the FLB condition, students played a game of floorball. The structure of the game was as follows, 2-minute instructions, 3-minute low-intensity warm-up followed by 3-minute high-intensity play and then 2-minutes of low-intensity play. They continued to switch between 3-minute high-intensity play and 2-minute low-intensity play for the duration of the game.

Related Article: Study Shows Active Children Are Better At Math

The children in the RUN condition participated in a similar protocol including instructions and warm up and alternating 3-minute high intensity running with 2-minute low intensity running. In both the FLB and RUN conditions, subjects completed 9 total minutes of high-intensity activity. Additionally, subjects in the CON condition were instructed to sit comfortably and had the opportunity to watch cartoons. Subjects HR was monitored using a Polar tea 2 system heart monitor.

After completing their designated intervention, participants all rested for 40-minutes. They then completed another motor memory task 1-hour, 24-hours, and 7-days post-intervention. The purpose of measuring performance at multiple points in time was to understand when the motor memory associated with the task was consolidated.

Motor Skills:

Baseline analyses revealed no significant difference between subjects’ motor skills prior to intervention. The results suggest that all students had comparable performance on the initial motor memory test.  Results revealed that engaging in an acute bout of exercise did indeed improve long-term motor memory. Interestingly, the positive effects were only present in the delayed testing that occurred 7-days post intervention (See Figure). Subjects in the RUN and the FLB groups both demonstrated improvements. These improvements suggest that the type of high-intensity training did not affect the outcome.

motorskills

Previous research has demonstrated that high-intensity exercise may diminish performance on memory tasks when the tasks are performed immediately or soon after the exercise intervention. There could be various reasons. For example, subjects may be overly excited after the exercise and thus unable to focus. The researchers of the present study assume that the increased performance 7-days post-intervention may be attributed to a longer memory consolidation process.

Related Article: Kids Need To Move To Improve

Is it possible that motor memory improved due to practice? That is, subjects experienced the motor memory task pre-intervention, 1-hour post, 24-hours post, and 7-days post. The control group did not experience the same improvements at 7-days post compared to those in the RUN and FLB groups. We can infer from this that the improvements were not due to practice effects and were caused by the high-intensity intervention.

Takeaways:

So what do these findings mean? The present research successfully builds upon previous work demonstrating the powerful cognitive benefits associated with high-intensity interval training. High-intensity training had positive effects on selective attention in school-age children.

Attention is the foundation for learning and memory. It is exciting to see this concept applied to a deeper cognitive process (attention vs. memory). From my point of view, the key takeaway is that intervals have the potential to improve selective attention and memory in children. The effects on memory, according to the present study, are more long-term and less immediate, however. As such, employing a steady regimen involving interval training could perhaps improve academic performance related to motor memory over time.

References:

Lundbye-Jensen, J., Skriver, K., Nielsen, JB., and Roig, M. (2017). Acute exercise improves motor memory consolidation in preadolescent children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11:182.

Ma, J.K., Mare, L.L., Gurd, B.J. (2015). Four minutes of in-class high-intensity interval activity improves selective attention in 9- to 11-year olds. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism, 40: 238-244.

Forever Fit Kids
Research
TAGGED : , , , ,

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *


Virginia Davies, CEO
“We the people” must come together and make some real change. Read this message from our CEO and social entrepreneur, Virginia Davies.